Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2012 Sep 4;109(36):14681-6.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1206608109. Epub 2012 Aug 20.

Lesion mapping of cognitive control and value-based decision making in the prefrontal cortex

Affiliations

Lesion mapping of cognitive control and value-based decision making in the prefrontal cortex

Jan Gläscher et al. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. .

Abstract

A considerable body of previous research on the prefrontal cortex (PFC) has helped characterize the regional specificity of various cognitive functions, such as cognitive control and decision making. Here we provide definitive findings on this topic, using a neuropsychological approach that takes advantage of a unique dataset accrued over several decades. We applied voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping in 344 individuals with focal lesions (165 involving the PFC) who had been tested on a comprehensive battery of neuropsychological tasks. Two distinct functional-anatomical networks were revealed within the PFC: one associated with cognitive control (response inhibition, conflict monitoring, and switching), which included the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex and a second associated with value-based decision-making, which included the orbitofrontal, ventromedial, and frontopolar cortex. Furthermore, cognitive control tasks shared a common performance factor related to set shifting that was linked to the rostral anterior cingulate cortex. By contrast, regions in the ventral PFC were required for decision-making. These findings provide detailed causal evidence for a remarkable functional-anatomical specificity in the human PFC.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Figures

Fig. 1.
Fig. 1.
Results from the lesion overlap analysis of different tests of cognitive control and value-based decision making. VLSM results are thresholded at P < 0.05 (FDR) and projected on a template brain in neurological convention (R = right; all midsaggital hemispheric views are left hemisphere). (A) Results for the TMT, STROOP, WCST, and COWA. (B) Results for the IGT. The graphs at Right show the mean test score (error bars = SEM) for the participants with lesions in the local maximum of each particular test compared with participants with lesions elsewhere. The respective local maximum is highlighted by the white and black circles on the overlay in the middle column. Grayed out areas show regions without sufficient statistical power to detect a lesion deficit effect (see Fig. S2 for complete power maps).
Fig. 2.
Fig. 2.
Results for all VLSM analyses projected onto a template brain in neurological convention (R = right). (A) All results are thresholded at P < 0.05 (FDR) and coded in different colors. (B) Overlap ratio [(Number of significant voxels in Test A and Test B) / (Number of significant voxels in either A or B)]. This ratio quantifies the volumetric overlap in those regions significant for one task relative to another task (minimum = 0, maximum = 1). Because of the different number of significant voxels in each test (base rate), the overlap matrix is asymmetrical. (C) Scatter plot (r = −0.37, P = 0.0001) of IGT performance and the extent to which individual lesions overlap with the vmPFC. Highlighted are patients who participated in other decision making tasks along with the IGT.
Fig. 3.
Fig. 3.
(A) Results of a VLSM analysis of the factor scores of a cognitive control factor. The loadings were computed by extracting a single factor using common factor analysis. (B) The cognitive control factor correlates with a region in the ACC that was significant for TMT and WCST (magnification of the saggital slice). (C) Difference image between IGT and the cognitive control factor scores. The images map out the mean differences between both z-scored variables highlighting a valuation network (i.e., patients with lower IGT scores) in blue and a cognitive control network (i.e., patients with lower executive factor scores) in red. (Top) Whole-brain reconstructions (with part of dorsal PFC cut away on the right to visualize internal details). (Middle and Bottom) Slices as indicated.

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Stuss DT, Levine B. Adult clinical neuropsychology: Lessons from studies of the frontal lobes. Annu Rev Psychol. 2002;53:401–433. - PubMed
    1. Miyake A, et al. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognit Psychol. 2000;41:49–100. - PubMed
    1. Norman DA, Shallice T. Attenion to action: Willed and automatic control of behavior. In: Davidson RJ, Schwartz GE, Shapiro D, editors. Consciousness and Self-Regulation: Advances in Research and Theory. Vol 4. New York: Plenum; 1986. pp. 1–18.
    1. Miller EK, Cohen JD. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2001;24:167–202. - PubMed
    1. Watanabe M. Role of anticipated reward in cognitive behavioral control. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2007;17:213–219. - PubMed

Publication types