Abstract
Background:
Cancer-related cognitive impairment (CRCI) is a common symptom in patients with breast cancer. In our previous study of 397 women with breast cancer, we identified 3 groups of patients with distinct CRCI profiles (ie, high, moderate, and low-moderate attentional function). Compared with the other 2 classes, the low-moderate class was younger, had more comorbidities, and with lower functional status.
Objectives:
In this study, we expand on this work and evaluate for differences among these latent classes in the severity of psychological (depression and anxiety) and physical (fatigue, decrements in energy, sleep disturbance, and pain) symptoms before surgery.
Methods:
Cancer-related cognitive impairment was assessed using the Attentional Functional Index from before through 6 months after surgery. Lower Attentional Functional Index scores indicate higher levels of CRCI. Psychological and physical symptoms were assessed with valid instruments. Parametric and nonparametric tests were used to evaluate for differences in symptom severity scores among the latent classes.
Results:
Approximately 60% of patients experienced CRCI (ie, moderate and low-moderate classes). Significant differences were found among the 3 classes in the severity of trait and state anxiety, depressive symptoms, fatigue, and sleep disturbance (ie, high < moderate < low-moderate). In addition, compared with the other 2 classes, the low-moderate class reported higher pain interference scores.
Conclusions:
These findings suggest that women with clinically meaningful levels of persistent CRCI have a relatively high symptom burden before surgery.
Implications for Practice:
Clinicians need to routinely perform preoperative assessments of CRCI and associated symptoms and initiate therapeutic interventions.
Keywords: Breast cancer, Cancer-related cognitive, impairment, Cognitive function, Depression, Fatigue, Sleep disturbance
Cancer-related cognitive impairment (CRCI) is defined as deficits in short-term and working memory, verbal fluency, processing speed, and attention span.1,2 Decrements in concentration and memory loss are two of the most common symptoms of CRCI.2 Approximately 30% to 35% of patients with breast cancer report CRCI before treatment, and 35% report that it persists after the completion of treatment.3–5 Cancer-related cognitive impairment has negative effects on patients’ abilities to make treatment decisions, adhere to therapy, and maintain optimal work performance.6
The fact that CRCI can occur before treatment suggests that inflammatory cytokines induced by the tumor itself trigger inflammatory processes and neuroanatomic changes (eg, white and gray matter loss5). However, across 6 cross-sectional studies,7–12 findings are inconsistent regarding presurgical risk factors for CRCI. Whereas in 3 studies7,9,11 no association was found with age, in 3 studies,8,10,12 younger patients reported worse cognitive function scores. In terms of education, whereas in 3 studies8,10,12 no association was found, in another study,9 lower levels of education were associated with higher levels of cognitive complaints.
In addition to demographic and clinical risk factors, a limited number of cross-sectional studies have evaluated for associations between CRCI and common psychological (ie, anxiety and depression) and physical (ie, fatigue, sleep disturbance, and pain) symptoms. Across the 3 studies that evaluated anxiety, whereas no associations were found in one study,10 in 2 studies,9,11 higher anxiety scores were associated with worse self-reported cognitive function scores. In terms of depressive symptoms, whereas one study found no association,10 in the other study,9 patients with higher levels of depressive symptoms reported more cognitive complaints. In the studies that evaluated for associations between sleep disturbance9 or physical fatigue,10 patients with higher levels of both of these symptoms reported more cognitive complaints. In 2 studies that used global measures of symptom distress and total mood disturbance,7,8 higher scores on both measures were associated with higher levels of cognitive dysfunction.
Only 3 longitudinal studies of CRCI in patients with breast cancer have included a presurgical assessment.13–15 In the first study that assessed CRCI from before through 24 months after surgery,13 CRCI worsened the first month after surgery but gradually improved and returned to presurgical levels at 12 months. Although no associations between demographic and clinical characteristics and CRCI were evaluated, at each assessment, higher levels of anxiety, depression, fatigue, and sleep disturbance were associated with worse self-report ratings of cognitive function. In the second study that assessed CRCI from before through 12 months after surgery,14 cognitive function scores increased slightly over time. The characteristics associated with poorer cognitive function scores included being non-White, having a higher level of comorbidity, receipt of adjuvant chemotherapy, and receipt of hormonal therapy. In addition, patients with higher levels of trait anxiety, fatigue, and sleep disturbance and lower levels of energy reported poorer cognitive function scores before surgery.
Although these 2 longitudinal studies13,14 provide useful information on changes in CRCI after breast cancer surgery, neither of them used a person-centered analytic approach to identify patients at higher risk for CRCI. In our previous analysis,15 we used growth mixture modeling to identify subgroups of patients with breast cancer (n = 397) with distinct CRCI profiles from before through 6 months after surgery, using the Attentional Functional Index (AFI)12 as the measure of CRCI. We identified 3 distinct CRCI profiles, namely, high (41.6%), moderate (25.4%), and low-moderate attentional function (33.0%). Compared with the high class, the low-moderate class was significantly younger. In addition, compared with the other 2 classes, the low-moderate class had a lower annual household income, a higher level of comorbidity, and a lower functional status. However, in this article, differences in presurgical psychological and physical symptom severity scores were not evaluated. Given that the findings from a recent review suggest that psychological and physical symptoms may co-occur and create a cascade effect that further worsens CRCI,16,17 as well as the paucity of research on co-occurring symptoms in patients with breast cancer with distinct CRCI profiles, the purpose of this study was to evaluate for differences among the 3 CRCI classes in the severity of psychological and physical symptoms before surgery.
Methods
This analysis is part of a larger, longitudinal study that evaluated multiple symptoms in patients who underwent breast cancer surgery.18 The theory of symptom management developed by faculty members at the University of California, San Francisco, served as the theoretical framework for the entire study.19 Patients were recruited from breast care centers in a Comprehensive Cancer Center, 2 public hospitals, and 4 community practices located in the greater San Francisco Bay area. Eligible patients were English-speaking women diagnosed with breast cancer, older than 18 years, scheduled to undergo surgery on 1 breast, and able to provide written informed consent. Patients scheduled to have surgery on both breasts and those with distant metastases at the time of diagnosis were excluded. Of the 516 patients who were approached, 410 enrolled in the study (79.5% response rate), and 397 completed the enrollment assessment. The most common reasons for refusal were being too busy or feeling overwhelmed.
Study Procedures
The study was approved by the Committee on Human Research at the University of California, San Francisco, and by the institutional review boards at each of the study sites. During preoperative visits, a clinical staff member explained the study and invited patients to participate. Patients who were willing to participate were introduced to a research nurse who determined their eligibility. After providing written informed consent, patients completed the enrollment questionnaires an average of 4 days before surgery. Follow-up questionnaires were completed each month for 6 months after surgery (ie, 7 assessments over 6 months).
Instruments
Demographic and Clinical Characteristics
Patients completed a demographic questionnaire, the Karnofsky Performance Status scale,20 and the Self-Administered Comorbidity Questionnaire.21 Medical records were reviewed for disease and treatment information.
Attentional Function
Changes in attentional function from before through 6 months after surgery were assessed using the AFI.12 This 16-item instrument assesses an individual’s perceived effectiveness in performing daily activities that are supported by attention and working memory in the past week. A higher total mean score on a 0 to 10 scale indicates greater capacity to direct attention. Total scores are grouped into categories of attentional function (ie, <5.0 low function, 5.0–7.5 moderate function, and >7.5 high function).8 In this study, Cronbach’s alpha for the total AFI score was .93.
Psychological Symptoms
ANXIETY
State and trait anxiety were assessed using the 20-item Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventories (STAI-S and STAI-T, respectively). Patients completed the items using the time frame of “right now.” Total scores for each scale range from 20 to 80, with higher scores indicating greater anxiety. Scores of 31.8 or higher and 32.2 or higher suggest high levels of trait and state anxiety, respectively.22 In this study, Cronbach’s alphas for the STAI-T and STAI-S were .88 and .95, respectively.
DEPRESSION
The 20-item Center for Epidemiologic Studies–Depression (CES-D) scale was used to assess depressive symptoms in the past week. Total scores can range from 0 to 60, with scores of 16 or higher indicating the need for clinical evaluation of depression. Four subscale scores (ie, somatic, depressed affect, positive affect, and interpersonal problems) were calculated.23 In this study, Cronbach’s alpha for the total CES-D score was .90.
Physical Symptoms
FATIGUE AND ENERGY
The 18-item Lee Fatigue Scale was designed to assess physical fatigue and energy.24 Each item was rated on a 0 to 10 numeric rating scale (NRS) using the time frame of “right now.” Total fatigue and energy scores were calculated as the mean of the 13 fatigue items and the 5 energy items, with higher scores indicating greater fatigue severity and higher levels of energy. Cutoff scores of 4.4 or higher and 4.8 or less indicate clinically meaningful levels of fatigue and decrements in energy levels, respectively.25 In this study, Cronbach’s alphas for the fatigue and energy scales were .96 and .93, respectively.
SLEEP DISTURBANCE
The 21-item General Sleep Disturbance Scale (GSDS) was designed to assess sleep disturbance in the past week. Each item was rated on a 0 (never) to 7 (everyday) NRS. The GSDS total score is the sum of the 7 subscale scores that can range from 0 (no disturbance) to 147 (extreme sleep disturbance). Each mean subscale score can range from 0 to 7. Higher total and subscale scores indicate higher levels of sleep disturbance.26 Subscales scores of 3 or higher and a GSDS total score of 43 or higher indicate a clinical meaningful level of sleep disturbance.27 In this study, Cronbach’s alpha for total GSDS score was .86.
PAIN
Breast pain was evaluated using the Breast Symptoms Questionnaire (BSQ). Part 1 of the BSQ obtained information on the occurrence of pain in the affected breast. Patients who reported breast pain were asked to complete part 2 of the BSQ, which assessed pain intensity “right now,” average daily pain, and worst pain using 0 (no pain) to 10 (worst imaginable pain) NRSs, as well as number of days per week with pain and number of hours per day in pain in the past week. Patients who reported breast pain rated its level of interference using a 0 (no interference) to 10 (complete interference) NRS. The 8 items that assessed pain interference were adapted from the interference scale of the Wisconsin Brief Pain Inventory.28 Eight additional items were used to assess pain interference based on studies by Tasmuth et al.29,30
Statistical Analyses
Data were analyzed using SPSS 27 (IBM, Armonk, New York) and Mplus 6.11 (Muthen and Muthen, Los Angeles, California). Descriptive statistics and frequency distributions were generated for sample characteristics and symptom scores. Growth mixture modeling with robust maximum likelihood estimation identified latent classes of patient with distinct trajectories of cognitive function. The growth mixture modeling methods are described in detail elsewhere.15,31
Parametric and nonparametric tests were used to evaluate for differences in demographic, clinical, and symptom characteristics among the classes. A value of P < .05 was considered statistically significant. Post hoc contrasts were done using a Bonferroni corrected P < .017 (.05/3 possible pairwise comparisons).
Results
Growth Mixture Modeling Classes
As described previously,15 among the 397 patients, 3 classes with distinct attentional function profiles were identified. Lower scores on the AFI indicate lower levels of attention and working memory or higher levels of CRCI. As illustrated in the Figure, patients in the high attentional function (“high”) class (41.6%) had an estimated AFI score of 7.78 at enrollment, which increased and remained high over the next 6 months. Patients in the moderate attentional function (“moderate”) class (25.4%) had an estimated AFI score of 6.58 at enrollment that decreased and then increased significantly but remained moderate over the next 6 months. Patients in the low-moderate attentional function (“low-moderate”) class (33.0%) had an estimated AFI score of 5.23 at enrollment that did not change significantly over the next 6 months.
Figure.
Observed and estimated attentional function latent classes from before through 6 months after breast cancer surgery. Adapted from Merriman JD, Aouizerat BE, Cataldo JK, et al. Association between an interleukin 1 receptor, type 1 promoter polymorphism and self-reported attentional function in women with breast cancer. Cytokine 2014;65:192–201.
Demographic and Clinical Characteristics
Compared with the high class, the low-moderate class was significantly younger and more likely to self-report a diagnosis of depression. Compared with the other 2 classes, the low-moderate class had a lower annual household income, a higher Self-Administered Comorbidity Questionnaire score, and a lower Karnofsky Performance Status score (Table 1).
Table 1 •.
Differences in Demographic and Clinical Characteristics Among the Attentional Function Classes at Enrollment
Characteristic | High Attentional Function (1) |
Moderate Attentional Function (2) |
Low-Moderate Attentional Function (3) |
Statistics |
---|---|---|---|---|
n= 165 (41.6%) |
n = 101 (25.4%) |
n = 131 (33.0%) |
||
Mean (SD) | Mean (SD) | Mean (SD) | ||
| ||||
Age y | 56.7 (11.2) | 55.2 (10.3) | 52.6 (12.6) | F = 4.87, P = .008 1 > 3 |
Education, y | 15.8 (2.7) | 15.8 (2.9) | 15.6 (2.4) | F = 0.25, P =.780 |
Self-Administered Comorbidity Questionnaire score | 3.8 (2.5) | 4.0 (2.6) | 5.1 (3.2) | F = 8.84, P <.001 1 and 2 < 3 |
Body mass index, kg/m2 | 25.9 (5.7) | 27.6 (6.6) | 27.4 (6.3) | F = 3.43, P =.033 |
No significant pairwise contrasts | ||||
Karnofsky Performance Status score | 95.7 (8.6) | 94.9 (6.9) | 88.8 (12.8) | F = 19.57, P < .001 1 and 2 > 3 |
n (%) | n (%) | n (%) | ||
Race/ethnicity | ||||
White | 113 (69.3) | 70 (69.3) | 72 (55.0) | χ2 = 9.46, P =.149 |
Black | 15 (9.2) | 7 (6.9) | 18 (13.7) | |
Asian/Pacific Islander | 17 (10.4) | 14 (13.9) | 19 (14.5) | |
Hispanic/ mixed/ other | 18 (11.0) | 10 (9.9) | 22 (16.8) | |
Live alone (% yes) | 39 (23.9) | 19 (19.2) | 36 (27.7) | χ2 = 2.23, P = .328 |
Married or partnered (% yes) | 69 (42.1) | 36 (36.0) | 60 (46.2) | χ2 = 2.40, P = .301 |
Currently employed (% yes) | 84 (51.5) | 52 (51.5) | 53 (40.8) | χ2 = 4.03, P = .133 |
Household income level | KW= 13.22, P =.001 | |||
<$30 000 | 19 (14.3) | 15 (16.7) | 36 (34.0) | 1 and 2 > 3 |
$30 000–$99 999 | 61 (45.9) | 33 (36.7) | 40 (37.7) | |
≥$100 000 | 53 (39.8) | 42 (47.6) | 30 (28.3) | |
Regular exercise (% yes) | 118 (71.5) | 74 (74.0) | 82 (63.6) | χ2 = 3.42, P =.181 |
Occurrence of comorbid conditions | ||||
Heart disease | 9 (5.5) | 1 (1.0) | 5 (3.8) | χ2 = 3.44, P =.179 |
High blood pressure | 54 (32.7) | 29 (28.7) | 40 (30.5) | χ2 = 0.49, P = .783 |
Lung disease | 4 (2.4) | 2 (2.0) | 6 (4.6) | χ2= 1.66, P = .436 |
Diabetes | 9 (5.5) | 8 (7.9) | 14 (10.7) | χ2 = 2.78, P = .249 |
Ulcer | 4 (2.4) | 1 (1.0) | 10 (7.6) | χ2 = 8.35, P =.015 |
No significant pairwise contrasts | ||||
Kidney disease | 2 (1.2) | 0 (0.0) | 1 (0.8) | χ2= 1.23, P = .541 |
Liver disease | 5 (3.0) | 1 (1.0) | 4 (3.1) | χ2= 1.29, P = .525 |
Anemia | 11 (6.7) | 8 (7.9) | 12 (9.2) | χ2 = 0.63, P = .729 |
Depression | 26 (15.8) | 22 (21.8) | 38 (29.0) |
χ2 = 7.56, P = .023 1 < 3 |
Osteoarthritis | 25 (15.2) | 19 (18.8) | 25 (19.1) | χ2 = 0.98, P =.613 |
Back pain | 40 (24.2) | 25 (24.8) | 46 (35.1) | χ2 = 4.98, P = .083 |
Rheumatoid arthritis | 3 (1.8) | 5 (5.0) | 6 (4.6) | χ2 = 2.45, P = .295 |
Postmenopausal (% yes) | 106 (65.4) | 64 (66.7) | 78 (60.9) | χ2 = 0.95, P =.621 |
Stage of disease | ||||
Stage 0 | 33 (20.0) | 18 (17.8) | 22 (16.8) | KW = 4.20, P =.122 |
Stage I | 67 (40.6) | 42 (41.6) | 42 (32.1) | |
Stage II | 54 (32.7) | 35 (34.7) | 52 (39.7) | |
Stage III and IV | 11 (6.7) | 6 (5.9) | 15 (11.5) | |
Receipt of neoadjuvant therapy (% yes) | 27 (16.4) | 17 (17.0) | 35 (26.7) | χ2 = 5.63, P =.060 |
HRT before surgery (% yes) | 22 (13.3) | 24 (24.0) | 21 (16.2) | χ2 = 5.12, P =.077 |
Type of surgery | ||||
Breast conservation | 135 (81.8) | 77 (76.2) | 106 (80.9) | χ2= 1.31, P = .521 |
Mastectomy | 30 (18.2) | 24 (23.8) | 25 (19.1) | |
Sentinel node biopsy (% yes) | 140 (84.8) | 88 (87.1) | 100 (76.3) | χ2 = 5.60, P =.061 |
Axillary lymph node dissection (% yes) | 52 (31.5) | 38 (38.0) | 58 (44.3) | χ2 = 5.10, P =.078 |
Receipt of adjuvant chemotherapy (% yes) a | 48 (29.1) | 40 (39.6) | 45 (34.3) | χ2 = 3.17, P =.205 |
Receipt of radiation therapy (% yes)a | 95 (57.6) | 52 (51.5) | 77 (58.8) | χ2 = 1.39, P =.500 |
Receipt of hormonal therapy (% yes) | 78 (47.3) | 38 (37.6) | 52 (39.7) | χ2 = 2.94, P =.230 |
Estrogen receptor positive (% positive) | 137 (83.5) | 74 (73.3) | 96 (73.3) | χ2 = 5.81, P =.055 |
Progesterone receptor positive (% positive) | 124 (75.6) | 70 (69.3) | 85 (64.9) | χ2 = 4.11, P =.128 |
HER2/neu (% positive) | 21 (14.3) | 15 (17.0) | 23 (18.7) | χ2 = 0.98, P =.614 |
Abbreviations: HER2/neu, human epidermal growth factor receptor; HRT, hormone replacement therapy; KW, Kruskal-Wallis test; SD, standard deviation.
Receipt of treatment in the 6 months after surgery.
Psychological Symptoms
Among the 3 AFI classes, differences in trait and state anxiety followed the same pattern (high < moderate < low-moderate). In terms of depressive symptoms, except for the CES-D interpersonal problems (high < low-Moderate) and CES-D positive affect (high > moderate > low-moderate) subscales, for all the other subscales and total CES-D scores, differences among the AFI classes followed the same pattern (high < moderate < low-moderate; Table 2).
Table 2 •.
Differences in Psychological and Physical Symptom Severity Scores Among the Attentional Function Classes at Enrollment
High Attentional Function (1) |
Moderate Attentional Function (2) |
Low-Moderate Attentional Function (3) |
||
---|---|---|---|---|
n = 165 (41.6%) |
n = 101 (25.4%) |
n = 131 (33.0%) |
||
Symptoms a | Mean (SD) | Mean (SD) | Mean (SD) | Statistics |
| ||||
Psychological symptoms | ||||
Trait anxiety (≥31.8) | 31.6 (7.4) | 34.9 (8.2) | 40.3 (9.2) | F = 38.86, P <.001 1 < 2 < 3 |
State anxiety (≥32.2) | 36.7 (12.7) | 42.9 (12.9) | 47.1 (12.4) | F = 24.60, P < .001 1 < 2 < 3 |
Center for Epidemiological Studies–Depression | ||||
Somatic subscale | 3.7 (2.9) | 5.9 (4.1) | 7.9 (3.7) | F = 51.10, P <.001 1 < 2 < 3 |
Depressed affect subscale | 3.3 (3.3) | 4.8 (4.2) | 6.9 (4.7) | F = 26.64, P < .001 1 < 2 < 3 |
Positive affect subscale | 10.0 (2.2) | 9.2 (2.7) | 7.9 (2.7) | F = 24.46, P < .001 1 > 2 > 3 |
Interpersonal problems subscale | 0.14 (0.67) | 0.23 (0.92) | 0.44 (0.89) | F = 4.70, P =.010 1 < 3 |
Total score (≥16.0) | 9.2 (7.1) | 13.7 (9.5) | 19.4 (9.6) | F = 49.72, P < .001 1 < 2 < 3 |
Physical symptoms | ||||
Lee Fatigue Scale-Fatigue (≥4.4) | 2.1 (2.0) | 3.2 (2.1) | 4.3 (2.3) | F = 35.51, P <.001 1 < 2 < 3 |
Lee Energy Scale-Energy (≤4.8) | 5.8 (2.6) | 4.6 (2.2) | 4.0 (2.2) | F = 22.56, P < .001 1 > 2 and 3 |
General Sleep Disturbance Scale | ||||
Medications for sleep (≥3.0) | 0.3 (0.6) | 0.5 (0.6) | 0.6 (0.8) | F = 4.21, P =.016 1 < 3 |
Quality of sleep (≥3.0) | 2.5 (1.9) | 3.5 (1.9) | 4.2 (1.7) | F = 30.81, P <.001 1 < 2 < 3 |
Quantity of sleep (≥3.0) | 4.6 (1.4) | 5.0 (1.6) | 5.0 (1.5) | F = 3.95, P =.020 1 < 3 |
Sleep onset latency (≥3.0) | 1.6 (2.0) | 2.3 (2.2) | 3.4 (2.5) | F = 22.48, P < .001 1 < 2 < 3 |
Mid sleep awakenings (≥3.0) | 3.9 (2.5) | 3.9 (2.3) | 4.7 (2.2) | F = 4.74, P =.009 1 and 2 < 3 |
Early awakenings (≥3.0) | 2.8 (2.5) | 3.2 (2.7) | 3.9 (2.4) | F = 6.46, P =.002 1 < 3 |
Excessive daytime sleepiness (≥3.0) | 1.4 (1.0) | 2.1 (1.1) | 2.8 (1.3) | F = 50.93, P <.001 1 < 2 < 3 |
Total score (≥43.0) | 38.3 (19.0) | 49.4 (19.1) | 59.5 (19.9) | F = 43.14, P < .001 |
Breast pain | n (%) | n (%) | n (%) | 1 < 2 < 3 |
Occurrence of pain in the affected breast before surgery (% yes) | 33 (20.5) | 33 (33.0) | 43 (33.6) |
χ2 = 7.72, P = .021 1 < 3 |
For patients with breast pain | Mean (SD) | Mean (SD) | Mean (SD) | |
Pain right now | 1.0 (1.4) | 1.8 (2.2) | 1.9 (2.1) | F = 2.26, P =.110 |
Current average daily pain | 1.5 (1.2) | 2.1 (2.2) | 2.7 (2.2) | F = 3.00, P =.055 |
Worst pain | 2.8 (1.6) | 3.1 (2.2) | 4.3 (2.6) | F = 4.44, P = .014 1 < 3 |
No. days per week in pain | 1.7 (2.3) | 3.0 (3.0) | 3.5 (2.6) | F = 4.45, P = .014 1 < 3 |
Breast Pain Interference | ||||
Brief Pain Inventoryb | 0.9 (1.9) | 1.2 (1.6) | 2.5 (2.4) | F = 6.36, P = .002 1 and 2 < 3 |
Additional interference items c | 0.5 (1.0) | 0.8 (1.3) | 1.8 (2.1) | F = 6.83, P = .002 1 and 2 < 3 |
All interference items | 0.7 (1.4) | 1.0 (1.3) | 2.1 (2.1) | F = 7.30, P = .001 1 and 2 < 3 |
Abbreviation: SD, standard deviation.
Numbers in parentheses indicate clinically meaningful cutoff scores.
Interference items from the Brief Pain Inventory: walking ability, normal work, mood, sexual activity, general activity, relations with other people, sleep, and enjoyment of life.
Additional interference items: ability to sleep on the affected side, ability to touch the site, ability to reach out in front of you, ability to carry things, ability to get up from bed, ability to do handicrafts, ability to drive a car, ability to write, and ability to reach above your head.
Physical Symptoms
Among the 3 AFI classes, levels of fatigue, worse sleep quality, sleep onset latency, excessive daytime sleepiness, and overall level of sleep disturbance followed the same pattern (high < moderate < low-moderate). Compared with the other 2 classes, the low-moderate class had higher scores for mid sleep awakenings and for all of the pain interference scores. Compared with the high class, the other 2 classes had worse decrements in energy. Compared with the high class, the low-moderate class had higher scores for use of medications for sleep, quantity of sleep (ie, fewer hours of sleep), early awakenings, worst pain, and number of days per week in pain, as well as a higher occurrence rate for breast pain before surgery (Table 2).
Discussion
This study extends our previous work15 by evaluating for differences in the severity of common psychological and physical symptoms before surgery in 3 groups of patients with breast cancer with distinct CRCI profiles. Compared with previous preoperative prevalence rates of 30% to 35%,3–5 almost 60% of our patients had decrements in cognitive function. Several plausible explanations exist for these inconsistent findings. First, the definitions for CRCI and criteria used to diagnose CRCI varied across studies. Second, a wide variety of subjective and/or objective measures were used to assess CRCI. Of note, subjective assessments, like the one used in our study, may be better able to detect early and more subtle forms of CRCI than objective measures.32 In addition, different analytic methods were used to estimate the prevalence rates for CRCI. Rather than use cutpoint or summary scores, in the current study, we used a person-centered analytic approach to identify patients with distinct CRCI profiles.
In our previous report,15 the demographic and clinical characteristics associated with latent class membership were described. This discussion focuses on differences among the classes in the severity of the common psychological and physical symptoms that were evaluated before surgery.
Psychological Symptoms
Consistent with previous reports,9,11,13,14 progressively higher levels of anxiety were associated with worse levels of cognitive function. Not surprising, for all 3 classes, state anxiety scores exceeded the clinically meaningful cutoffs. Anxiety increases when individuals encounter high levels of threat like a cancer diagnosis and impending surgery. According to the theory of attentional control, increased anxiety may decrease one’s ability to flexibly focus and shift attention to current goals, which impairs working memory and allocation of attentional resources.33,34 Our findings suggest that early assessments of preoperative levels of anxiety and appropriate interventions warrant consideration.35
Similar to anxiety and consistent with previous reports,9,13 progressively higher levels of depressive symptoms were associated with worse levels of cognitive function. Whereas the moderate class reported subsyndromal levels of depressive symptoms (ie, 13.7),36,37 the low-moderate class had total CES-D scores (ie, 19.4) that warrant clinical evaluation. In addition, compared with the other 2 classes, the low-moderate class reported higher somatic symptom and lower positive affect scores. These findings suggest a possible interdependence between somatic symptoms and negative affect. One possible explanation for this interdependence is that external stressors (eg, cancer diagnosis) activate the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis, which leads to decreased activity in the prefrontal cortex as a result of changes in serotonin metabolism.38 In turn, these changes impair one’s ability to control negative elaborative processes (eg, rumination) and results in increased depressive symptoms and an exacerbation of negative affect.38 Consistent with previous reports in older, community-dwelling adults39 and patients with breast cancer after the initiation of chemotherapy,40 compared with the high class, the low-moderate class reported higher interpersonal problems. One possible explanation for this finding is that social and family support may act as buffers for depressive symptoms and partially moderate the association between depressed affect and CRCI.41–43
Physical Symptoms
Comparable to previous reports of fatigue in patients with breast cancer before surgery,10,13 33% of patients in the low-moderate class reported fatigue scores that approached the clinically meaningful cutoff. One possible explanation for the co-occurrence of fatigue and CRCI is that the tumor itself triggers the release of proinflammatory cytokines.5,44 Given that our previous work demonstrated that fatigue and energy are distinct but related symptoms,45,46 it is interesting to note that almost 60% of our sample reported clinically meaningful decrements in energy. Given that this study is the first to report on this association, additional research is warranted on diurnal variations in both fatigue and energy to confirm those findings and evaluate for underlying mechanisms.
Consistent with a study of patients undergoing surgery for lung cancer,47 almost 60% of our patients reported clinically meaningful levels of sleep disturbance before surgery. The total GSDS scores reported by the low-moderate class are higher than those reported by postpartum mothers (ie, 55.5),48 equivalent to those reported by permanent night shift workers (ie, 60.5),26 but lower than those reported by patients with non–central nervous system cancer (ie, 74.4)49 or breast cancer during chemotherapy (ie, 69.3).50 An evaluation of the GSDS subscale scores provides insights into the types of sleep disturbance our patients were experiencing. All 3 classes reported clinically meaningful decrements in the quantity of their sleep and a higher level of midsleep awakenings. However, whereas the moderate class reported problems primarily with sleep maintenance (ie, higher scores for early and mid-sleep awakenings), the low-moderate class had problems with both sleep initiation (ie, longer sleep onset latency) and sleep maintenance. Of note, the use of sleep medications was very low across all 3 groups.
Our association between higher levels of sleep disturbance and worse cognitive function is consistent with previous studies.9,13 For example, in one study of older adults in the general population,51 an inverted U-shaped association was found between sleep quantity and cognitive decline. The authors noted that compared with individuals who had an adequate number of hours of sleep (7 hours), cognitive decline was faster in those individuals who had an insufficient (<4 hours) or excessive (>10 hours) sleep duration per night. In another study that examined the sleep-wake patterns of elderly women,52 sleep fragmentation and longer average sleep latency were associated with neurodegeneration and cognitive decline. Given that sleep plays an important role in memory consolidation53 and restorative process,54 clinicians need to assess for this symptom and initiate appropriate interventions (eg, sleep hygiene, medication, cognitive-behavioral therapy).55
Although only 30% of the women had pain in their breast before surgery, the low-moderate class had worst pain intensity scores in the moderate range. Based on our previous analyses,56,57 this preoperative breast pain may be related to inflammatory changes associated with the total number and/or timing of the previous breast biopsies. In terms of its association with CRCI, in one conceptual model of pain-related cognitive impairment,58 pain may have negative effects on cognition through changes in brain networks (eg, prefrontal cortex, hippocampus, amygdala). Additional research is warranted on the effects of acute and chronic pain on CRCI in women before and after breast cancer surgery.
In summary, for the majority of the symptoms, the severity scores increased in a stepwise fashion across the CRCI classes. These findings suggest additive or synergistic interactions among these symptoms and that they may share common underlying mechanisms. For example, the default mode network (DMN), a brain network composed of the medial prefrontal cortex, the posterior cingulate cortex, inferior parietal lobule, hippocampus, and the precuneus, are all associated with cognitive function (eg, implicit learning, memory retrieval, prospection).59–61 Findings from functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies suggest that the DMN is deactivated when attention is focused on the external environment and during the formation of working memory. In contrast, it is highly activated when a person is not engaging with specific behavioral tasks and in self-referential processing.61,62 In a recent fMRI study,63 compared with nonfatigue patients, fatigued patients with breast cancer had increased DMN activity. In addition, decreased connectivity between the DMN and other brain structures was associated with higher levels of depressive symptoms in patients with breast cancer receiving chemotherapy. Taken together, these fMRI findings suggest that the co-occurrence of these symptoms may be the result of prolonged activation of the DMN network.
Another possible explanation for these findings is that the release of tumor-induced inflammatory cytokines leads to increased concentrations of various neurotransmitters (eg, serotonin, dopamine).5 Alterations in levels of these neurotransmitters contribute to the development of these common symptoms.5 In recent studies,64,65 whereas higher levels of IL-6 and serotonin were associated with increases in DMN activity, increases in dopamine had the opposite effect on DMN activity. Future research needs to examine the interrelationships among various biomarkers, DMN activity, CRCI, and other common symptoms to increase our understanding of their common and distinct underlying mechanisms.
Limitations
Although this longitudinal study has numerous strengths including a relatively large sample size and a comprehensive evaluation of associations between common symptoms and CRCI, several limitations need to be acknowledged. First, most of the women were well educated and diagnosed with early-stage breast cancer, which limits the generalizability of our findings. Second, our assessment of CRCI was based on an instrument that primarily evaluates attention and executive function. Although some studies have included healthy controls,10,66 this study aimed to identify groups of patients with breast cancer with distinct CRCI profiles from before through 6 months after breast cancer surgery.
Implications for Research and Practice
Future studies need to evaluate for distinct CRCI profiles, using both subjective and objective measures in patients with other types of cancer who are undergoing different types of cancer treatment. In addition, longitudinal studies that use analytic techniques like parallel process growth modeling67 are needed to determine which symptom(s) are driving or influencing the severity of the other common symptoms. These types of studies will aid in the development and testing of interventions for CRCI.
Given the high rates of CRCI, as well as the clinically meaningful levels of common physical and psychological symptoms before surgery, clinicians need to perform comprehensive symptom assessments and initiate appropriate pharmacologic and nonpharmacologic interventions. Nonpharmacologic interventions that are effective for multiple symptoms (eg, mindfulness-based stress reduction, cognitive-behavioral therapy) can be prescribed.2,68 Some patients may warrant referrals to psychological or symptom management services.
Acknowledgments
This study was funded by grants from the National Cancer Institute (CA107091 and CA118658). Dr Christine Makowski is an American Cancer Society Clinical Research Professor. This project was supported by the National Institutes of Health/National Center for Research Resources, University of California San Francisco–Clinical and Translational Science Institute, grant number ULI RR024131. Its contents are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health. Ms Allemann-Su received funding from the European Union Horizon 2020, Marie Sklodowska-Curie Research and Innovation Award (no. 801076), and the Swiss Cancer League (KLS-4294-08-2017).
Footnotes
The authors have no conflicts of interest to disclose.
Contributor Information
Yu-Yin Allemann-Su, Department of Clinical Research, University of Basel.
Marcus Vetter, Department of Oncology, Cantonal Hospital Basel-Land, Liestal.
Helen Koechlin, Faculty of Psychology, University of Basel, Switzerland; Department of Anaesthesiology, Boston Children’s Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts.
Yvette Conley, School of Nursing, University of Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania.
Steven M. Paul, School of Nursing, University of California, San Francisco..
Bruce A. Cooper, School of Nursing, University of California, San Francisco..
Kord M. Kober, School of Nursing, University of California, San Francisco..
Jon D. Levine, School of Medicine, University of California, San Francisco..
Christine Miaskowski, School of Nursing, University of California, San Francisco.; School of Medicine, University of California, San Francisco.
Maria C. Katapodi, Department of Clinical Research, University of Basel.
References
- 1.Bai L, Yu E. A narrative review of risk factors and interventions for cancer-related cognitive impairment. Ann Transl Med. 2021;9(1):72. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Lange M, Joly F, Vardy J, et al. Cancer-related cognitive impairment: an update on state of the art, detection, and management strategies in cancer survivors. Ann Oncol. 2019;30(12):1925–1940. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Dijkshoorn ABC, van Stralen HE, Sloots M, Schagen SB, Visser-Meily JMA, Schepers VPM. Prevalence of cognitive impairment and change in patients with breast cancer: a systematic review of longitudinal studies. Psychooncology. 2021;30(5):635–648. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Janelsins MC, Kesler SR, Ahles TA, Morrow GR. Prevalence, mechanisms, and management of cancer-related cognitive impairment. Int Rev Psychiatry. 2014;26(1):102–113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Olson B, Marks DL. Pretreatment cancer-related cognitive impairment mechanisms and outlook. Cancer. 2019;11(5). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Boykoff N, Moieni M, Subramanian SK. Confronting chemobrain: an in-depth look at survivors’ reports of impact on work, social networks, and health care response. J Cancer Surviv. 2009;3(4):223–232. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Cimprich B Pretreatment symptom distress in women newly diagnosed with breast cancer. Cancer Nurs. 1999;22(3):185–194. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Cimprich B, So H, Ronis DL, Trask C. Pre-treatment factors related to cognitive functioning in women newly diagnosed with breast cancer. Psychooncology. 2005;14(1):70–78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Hardy-Leger I, Charles C, Lange M, et al. Differentiation of groups of patients with cognitive complaints at breast cancer diagnosis: results from a sub-study of the French CANTO cohort. Psychooncology. 2021;30(4):463–470. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Lange M, Hardy-Léger I, Licaj I, et al. Cognitive impairment in patients with breast cancer before surgery: results from a CANTO cohort subgroup. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev. 2020;29(9):1759–1766. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Lehto RH, Cimprich B. Anxiety and directed attention in women awaiting breast cancer surgery. Oncol Nurs Forum. 1999;26(4):767–772. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Cimprich B, Visovatti M, Ronis DL. The Attentional Function Index—a self-report cognitive measure. Psychooncology. 2011;20(2):194–202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Chen ML, Miaskowski C, Liu LN, Chen SC. Changes in perceived attentional function in women following breast cancer surgery. Breast Cancer Res Treat. 2012;131(2):599–606. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Kohler C, Chang M, Allemann-Su YY, et al. Changes in attentional function in patients from before through 12 months after breast cancer surgery. J Pain Symptom Manage. 2020;59(6):1172–1185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Merriman JD, Aouizerat BE, Cataldo JK, et al. Association between an interleukin 1 receptor, type I promoter polymorphism and self-reported attentional function in women with breast cancer. Cytokine. 2014;65(2):192–201. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Ahles TA, Root JC. Cognitive effects of cancer and cancer treatments. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2018;14:425–451. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Yang Y, Hendrix CC. Cancer-related cognitive impairment in breast cancer patients: influences of psychological variables. Asia Pac J Oncol Nurs. 2018;5(3):296–306. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.McCann B, Miaskowski C, Koetters T, et al. Associations between pro- and anti-inflammatory cytokine genes and breast pain in women prior to breast cancer surgery. J Pain. 2012;13(5):425–437. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Humphreys J, Janson S, Donesky D, et al. A middle range theory of symptom management. In: Smith MJ, Liehr PR, eds. Middle Range Theory in Nursing. 3rd ed. New York, NY: Springer Publishing Company; 2014:141–164. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Karnofsky D Performance Scale. New York, NY: Plenum Press; 1977. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Sangha O, Stucki G, Liang MH, Fossel AH, Katz JN. The Self-Administered Comorbidity Questionnaire: a new method to assess comorbidity for clinical and health services research. Arthritis Rheum. 2003;49(2):156–163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Spielberger C, Gorsuch R, Lushene P, Vaggs P, Jacobs G. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Index (Form Y). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press; 1983. [Google Scholar]
- 23.Sheehan TJ, Fifield J, Reisine S, Tennen H. The measurement structure of the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale. J Pers Assess. 1995;64(3):507–521. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Lee KA, Hicks G, Nino-Murcia G. Validity and reliability of a scale to assess fatigue. Psychiatry Res. 1991;36(3):291–298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Dhruva A, Dodd M, Paul SM, et al. Trajectories of fatigue in patients with breast cancer before, during, and after radiation therapy. Cancer Nurs. 2010;33(3):201–212. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Lee KA. Self-reported sleep disturbances in employed women. Sleep. 1992;15(6):493–498. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Fletcher BS, Paul SM, Dodd MJ, et al. Prevalence, severity, and impact of symptoms on female family caregivers of patients at the initiation of radiation therapy for prostate cancer. J Clin Oncol. 2008;26(4):599–605. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Cleeland CS, Ryan KM. Pain assessment: global use of the Brief Pain Inventory. Ann Acad Med Singap. 1994;23(2):129–138. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Tasmuth T, von Smitten K, Kalso E. Pain and other symptoms during the first year after radical and conservative surgery for breast cancer. Br J Cancer. 1996;74(12):2024–2031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Tasmuth T, von Smitten K, Hietanen P, Kataja M, Kalso E. Pain and other symptoms after different treatment modalities of breast cancer. Ann Oncol. 1995;6(5):453–459. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Dunn LB, Aouizerat BE, Cooper BA, et al. Trajectories of anxiety in oncology patients and family caregivers during and after radiation therapy. Eur J Oncol Nurs. 2012;16(1):1–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Jia M, Zhang X, Wei L, Gao J. Measurement, outcomes and interventions of cognitive function after breast cancer treatment: a narrative review. Asia Pac J Clin Oncol. 2021;17(4):321–329. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Eysenck MW, Derakshan N, Santos R, Calvo MG. Anxiety and cognitive performance: attentional control theory. Emotion. 2007;7(2):336–353. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Shi R, Sharpe L, Abbott M. A meta-analysis of the relationship between anxiety and attentional control. Clin Psychol Rev. 2019;72:101754. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Tulloch I, Rubin JS. Assessment and management of preoperative anxiety. J Voice. 2019;33(5):691–696. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Dunn LB, Cooper BA, Neuhaus J, et al. Identification of distinct depressive symptom trajectories in women following surgery for breast cancer. Health Psychol. 2011;30(6):683–692. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Judd LL, Paulus MP, Wells KB, Rapaport MH. Socioeconomic burden of subsyndromal depressive symptoms and major depression in a sample of the general population. Am J Psychiatry. 1996;153(11):1411–1417. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.De Raedt R, Koster EH. Understanding vulnerability for depression from a cognitive neuroscience perspective: a reappraisal of attentional factors and a new conceptual framework. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2010;10(1):50–70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Hill NL, Mogle J, Wion R, et al. Subjective cognitive impairment and affective symptoms: a systematic review. Gerontologist. 2016;56(6):e109–e127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Kim HJ, Jung SO, Kim H, Abraham I. Systematic review of longitudinal studies on chemotherapy-associated subjective cognitive impairment in cancer patients. Psychooncology. 2020;29(4):617–631. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Brandao T, Schulz MS, Matos PM. Psychological adjustment after breast cancer: a systematic review of longitudinal studies. Psychooncology. 2017;26(7):917–926. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Hu J, Wang X, Guo S, et al. Peer support interventions for breast cancer patients: a systematic review. Breast Cancer Res Treat. 2019;174(2):325–341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Parmelee Streck B, LoBiondo-Wood G. A systematic review of dyadic studies examining depression in couples facing breast cancer. J Psychosoc Oncol. 2020;38(4):463–480. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Bower JE, Ganz PA. Symptoms: fatigue and cognitive dysfunction. Adv Exp Med Biol. 2015;862:53–75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Eshragh J, Dhruva A, Paul SM, et al. Associations between neurotransmitter genes and fatigue and energy levels in women after breast cancer surgery. J Pain Symptom Manage. 2017;53(1):67–84.e7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Kober KM, Smoot B, Paul SM, Cooper BA, Levine JD, Miaskowski C. Polymorphisms in cytokine genes are associated with higher levels of fatigue and lower levels of energy in women after breast cancer surgery. J Pain Symptom Manage. 2016;52(5):695–708.e4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Halle IH, Westgaard TK, Wahba A, Oksholm T, Rustøen T, Gjeilo KH. Trajectory of sleep disturbances in patients undergoing lung cancer surgery: a prospective study. Interact Cardiovasc Thorac Surg. 2017;25(2):285–291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Gay CL, Lee KA, Lee SY. Sleep patterns and fatigue in new mothers and fathers. Biol Res Nurs. 2004;5(4):311–318. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Tejada M, Viele C, Kober KM, et al. Identification of subgroups of chemotherapy patients with distinct sleep disturbance profiles and associated co-occurring symptoms. Sleep. 2019;42(10). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Langford DJ, Paul SM, Cooper B, et al. Comparison of subgroups of breast cancer patients on pain and co-occurring symptoms following chemotherapy. Support Care Cancer. 2016;24(2):605–614. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Ma Y, Liang L, Zheng F, Shi L, Zhong B, Xie W. Association between sleep duration and cognitive decline. JAMA Netw Open. 2020;3(9):e2013573. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Diem SJ, Blackwell TL, Stone KL, et al. Measures of sleep-wake patterns and risk of mild cognitive impairment or dementia in older women. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2016;24(3):248–258. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Tononi G, Cirelli C. Sleep and the price of plasticity: from synaptic and cellular homeostasis to memory consolidation and integration. Neuron. 2014;81(1):12–34. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Frank MG, Heller HC. The function(s) of sleep. Handb Exp Pharmacol. 2019;253:3–34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Zeichner SB, Zeichner RL, Gogineni K, Shatil S, Ioachimescu O. Cognitive behavioral therapy for insomnia, mindfulness, and yoga in patients with breast cancer with sleep disturbance: a literature review. Breast Cancer (Auckl). 2017;11:1178223417745564. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Langford DJ, Schmidt B, Levine JD, et al. Preoperative breast pain predicts persistent breast pain and disability after breast cancer surgery. J Pain Symptom Manage. 2015;49(6):981–994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Miaskowski C, Paul SM, Cooper B, et al. Identification of patient subgroups and risk factors for persistent arm/shoulder pain following breast cancer surgery. Eur J Oncol Nurs. 2014;18(3):242–253. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Moriarty O, McGuire BE, Finn DP. The effect of pain on cognitive function: a review of clinical and preclinical research. Prog Neurobiol. 2011;93(3):385–404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Kesler SR. Default mode network as a potential biomarker of chemotherapy-related brain injury. Neurobiol Aging. 2014;35(Suppl 2):S11–S19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Buckner RL. The brain’s default network: origins and implications for the study of psychosis. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2013;15(3):351–358. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Smallwood J, Bernhardt BC, Leech R, Bzdok D, Jefferies E, Margulies DS. The default mode network in cognition: a topographical perspective. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2021;22(8):503–513. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Anticevic A, Repovs G, Shulman GL, Barch DM. When less is more: TPJ and default network deactivation during encoding predicts working memory performance. Neuroimage. 2010;49(3):2638–2648. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Hampson JP, Zick SM, Khabir T, Wright BD, Harris RE. Altered resting brain connectivity in persistent cancer related fatigue. Neuroimage Clin. 2015;8:305–313. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Conio B, Martino M, Magioncalda P, et al. Opposite effects of dopamine and serotonin on resting-state networks: review and implications for psychiatric disorders. Mol Psychiatry. 2020;25(1):82–93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Marsland AL, Kuan DC, Sheu LK, et al. Systemic inflammation and resting state connectivity of the default mode network. Brain Behav Immun. 2017;62:162–170. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Jung MS, Zhang M, Askren MK, et al. Cognitive dysfunction and symptom burden in women treated for breast cancer: a prospective behavioral and fMRI analysis. Brain Imaging Behav. 2017;11(1):86–97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Jung T, Wickrama KAS. An introduction to latent class growth analysis and growth mixture modeling. Soc Personality Psychology Compass. 2008;2(1):302–317. [Google Scholar]
- 68.Mayo SJ, Lustberg M, Dhillon HM, et al. Cancer-related cognitive impairment in patients with non-central nervous system malignancies: an overview for oncology providers from the MASCC neurological complications study group. Support Care Cancer. 2021;29(6):2821–2840. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]